International Journal of Mental Health ISSN: 0020-7411 (Print) 1557-9328 (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/mimh20 Images of the "Insane," the "Mentally Ill," and the "Depressed" in Nouméa, New Caledonia Benjamin Goodfellow , Laurent Defromont , Fanny Calandreau & Jean-Luc Roelandt To cite this article: Benjamin Goodfellow , Laurent Defromont , Fanny Calandreau & Jean-Luc Roelandt (2010) Images of the "Insane," the "Mentally Ill," and the "Depressed" in Nouméa, New Caledonia, International Journal of Mental Health, 39:1, 82-98 To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.2753/IMH0020-7411390105 Published online: 08 Dec 2014. Submit your article to this journal Article views: 9 View related articles Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at http://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=mimh20 Download by: [Laurentian University] Date: 18 March 2016, At: 19:45 82 International Journal of Mental Health International Journal of Mental Health, vol. 39, no. 1, Spring 2010, pp. 82–98. © 2010 M.E. Sharpe, Inc. All rights reserved. ISSN 0020–7411 / 2010 $9.50 + 0.00. DOI 10.2753/IMH0020-7411390105 Downloaded by [Laurentian University] at 19:45 18 March 2016 Benjamin Goodfellow, Laurent Defromont, Fanny Calandreau, and Jean-Luc Roelandt Images of the “Insane,” the “Mentally Ill,” and the “Depressed” in Nouméa, New Caledonia A Mental Health Survey in the General Population ABSTRACT: Social representations of mental health in the population of Nouméa and its suburbs (Greater Nouméa) in New Caledonia were studied during the third phase of the survey “Mental Health in the General Population: Images and Realities,” conducted in August 2006. Method: A representative sample of 904 participants taken from the population of Nouméa and Greater Nouméa were interviewed using the survey questionnaire. Analytical results were interpreted in the light of the results of the preceding survey, the specialized literature, and the cultural and social context in New Caledonia. Results: Data for the social representation of the “insane” person revealed dehumanizing aspects related to a lack of human features, incomprehensible behavior, and danger. The social representation of the mentally ill person Benjamin Goodfellow is a psychiatrist in the Georges Pompidou European Public Hospital Emergency Department in Paris and a collaborator of the French WHO Collaborating Center (WHOCC) for research and training in the field of mental health in Lille, France. Laurent Defromont is a psychiatrist in the public health service, head of the Information and Medical Research Department (Département d’Information et de Recherche Médicale) of the Lille City Public Mental Health Center (Etablissement Public de Santé Mentale) and a collaborator of the French WHOCC for research and training in the field of mental health in Lille, France. Fanny Calandreau is a psychiatrist in the public health service, head of out-patient care in the General Psychiatry Department, Albert Bousquet Hospital in Nouméa, New Caledonia, and a collaborator of the WHOCC for the survey “Mental Health in the General Population: Images and Realities.” Jean-Luc Roelandt is a psychiatrist in the public health service, head of sector 59G21 of public psychiatry in Lille, France, and head of the French WHOCC for research and training in the field of mental health in Lille, France. 82 Spring 2010 83 Downloaded by [Laurentian University] at 19:45 18 March 2016 was found to be a medicalized transition of the insane person, bearing more human and comprehensible aspects similar to those observed in France. Conclusions: The social representation of the depressed person comprise cultural variants and may be a promising gateway for campaigns to promote mental health. The social representation of the insane person comprises cultural invariants that are more archaic and probably involve universal features of human disposition (i.e., fear of the unknown in someone who violates the norms of human behavior). These variations may be essential to the understanding of exclusionary attitudes. Historians, philosophers, scientists, and psychiatric practitioners have recently started focusing on the exclusion of people suffering from mental disorders. This theme is a main preoccupation of the Mental Health in the General Population (MHGP) survey [1], as one of its aims is to promote mental health. We analyze the representations of the general population related to exclusionary attitudes toward people suffering from mental disorders in the socioanthropological section of the survey. Many authors highlight the relation between a population’s knowledge of mental health and their attitudes toward people believed to have a mental disorder.1 In general, lay knowledge, discourse, behaviors, and attitudes concerning mental health and people identified as suffering from mental health problems are very heterogeneous. Star [2, 3] reported that the populations she studied appear to have a good, although heterogeneous, knowledge of mental health that varied within the population surveyed and among the geographic areas concerned but that coexisted with stereotyped images and frightening archetypes implicitly although strongly attached to the mentally ill. Pénochet and Guimelli [4] suggested that the positive human aspect plays a central role, while the negative, inhuman, and dangerous aspect plays a peripheral role in the structure of the representations. This assumption remains to be confirmed, but there is apparently a clearly stratified structure to the thinking, attitudes, beliefs, and behaviors related to the vast fields of mental health, insanity, and psychiatry. There appears to be no interchange or communication between the strata. This heterogeneity could explain why it is so difficult to find a single common word to use when talking about mental health. The Concept of Social Representation According to Jodelet [5], a social representation is a “socially elaborated and shared form of knowledge that has a practical aim and plays a role in constructing a social group’s reality” (p. 53; see also [6]). This concept was introduced by Moscovici [7] in 1961, inspired by the works of Durkheim. Its complexity arises from the fact that it spans many fields of human science. The main difficulty in understanding a social representation, however, is that it is both a psychological and a social concept and therefore combines individual and collective dimensions. One particular field that has specialized in its study is social psychology. Downloaded by [Laurentian University] at 19:45 18 March 2016 84 International Journal of Mental Health To understand the implications of the concept social representation, it is worth examining Jodelet’s [6] definition in detail. A social representation is “socially elaborated and shared,” meaning that it is created and is necessarily transformed whenever an interaction or an exchange occurs (i.e., when language is used to communicate) (p. 53). It also means that it is not only the sum of several individuals’ representations but only exists on a collective scale. Social representation defines a collectivity in that the members of a social group can recognize each other through it. The form implies that a social representation is a semiotic concept; it takes the place of something else. It signifies or designates something by the particular form it takes, just as the shapes of letters form a word. It is a piece of knowledge, which means that it is definite, not questionable, like reality. It has a practical aim, which implies that it is used to exert an action on the environment or on other people. Only unquestionable knowledge can be used for action, which requires an absence of doubt. The social representation “plays a role in constructing a . . . reality” in that it is knowledge (p. 53). Jodelet provided an example that illustrates the point. At the beginning of the AIDS epidemic, before scientists provided any clear, reliable information about the virus, many lay theories were put forward concerning its origin and how it was transmitted. These lay theories appear to have led to attitudes of exclusion that were often inappropriate to the disease. This “reality” is a social group’s reality. It relates to what has been said before and implies that a group, a collectivity, or a sum of individuals must share the same reality to be defined as a social group. This reality is constructed using—among other elements—social representations. Study Site New Caledonia is a set of Melanesian islands and archipelagoes in the South Pacific Ocean. According to Kirch [8], it was settled in 1,000 b.c.e. The first inhabitants were descendants of the Austronesians who left Taiwan at least 500 years earlier. These people then populated all the remaining islands in the Pacific Ocean. According to Angleviel [9], New Caledonia has been known to Europeans since its discovery by J. Cook in 1774, and it officially became French in 1853. New Caledonia was successively a penitentiary, a settlement, and a mining colony, and its main economic activity is now nickel mining. According to Angleviel [9] and Barbançon [10], slightly more than one-half of the population is composed of Kanaks (i.e., descendants of the first Austronesians); the remainder is composed of Europeans, that is, descendants of convicts and of the French colonial and penitential staff, or recent Europeans immigrants, most of whom were born in France, as well as descendants of the black birding communities (i.e., the regional workforce hired at the end of the nineteenth century to build the colony)—most of whom came from New Hebrides, Indonesia, Wallis and Futuna Islands—and finally, recent immigrants from the Pacific, China, and Vietnam. Spring 2010 85 Method Downloaded by [Laurentian University] at 19:45 18 March 2016 Sample and Questionnaire The representative sample was comprised of 904 people taken from the population of Nouméa and its suburbs, Païta, Dumbea, and Mont Dore (i.e., Greater Nouméa; total population = 146,000). The sample was constructed using the quota sampling method, with data taken from the New Caledonian Institute of Statistics and Economic Studies (Institut de la Statistique et des Etudes Economiques de Nouvelle Calédonie: ISEE) and from the last census (in 1996), adjusted for the time of the survey. This survey was part of the general MHGP [1] survey, Nouméa being one of its third-phase sites. For a more detailed description of the method used in the present study, see the companion part of this work [11], which describes the epidemiology of mental disorders in Nouméa and Greater Nouméa investigated in the same survey. Three open-answer questions (whose answers are analyzed herein) were included in the socioanthropological section of the survey questionnaire and were asked at the beginning of the interview, following a general presentation of the goals and methods of the study during which the interviewees were informed that they would remain anonymous and after their informed consent was obtained. The three questions (asked in French) were: “In your opinion, what is an insane person?” “In your opinion, what is a mentally ill person?” and “In your opinion, what is a depressed person?” The three questions were asked in random order to avoid any statistically significant interference between the questions. The answers were recorded word for word by the interviewers, who had previously been trained in a three-day session. The three items, “insane person,” “mentally ill person,” and “depressed person” (in French), came up in a preliminary survey based on unstructured interviews in a test population selected from a range of international sites covered by the MHGP survey. Data collected during the interviews were analyzed by an expert committee including various specialists. After a number of validation steps, the socioanthropological questionnaire was constructed using questions concerning three archetypes called the “insane person,” the “mentally ill person,” and the “depressed person.” Analysis of Answers Using ALCESTE Analysis of the answers given by the 904 interviewees (2,695 answers were taken into account) required a statistical tool that could summarize the answers in a comprehensible way. Accordingly, we used the Analyse des Lexèmes Cooccurrents dans un Ensemble de Segments de Texte (Analysis of Lexemes that Co-Occur in a Set of Text Segments; ALCESTE) program [12, 13]. It reveals words or lexemes (i.e., the grammatical root of the words that co-occur in text segments). According to Reinert [13], the co-occurrence of particular words in context units (the text segments) 86 International Journal of Mental Health Figure 1 Hierarchical Descending Classification Cl. 1 ( 494uce) + Cl. 2 ( 357uce) + + Cl. 3 ( 744uce) + Cl. 4 (1082uce) + + Downloaded by [Laurentian University] at 19:45 18 March 2016 + obeys certain rules, and these rules are determined by the person who speaks. The ALCESTE method is based on the hypothesis that when people talk about a topic, they associate particular words in a particular way. By analyzing co-occurrences of particular words in particular context units, the program defines a set of classes or an arborescence of classes that becomes increasingly narrow. The entire text is initially considered as a single theme (i.e., the subject of the text). However, minor themes can be revealed in the text. ALCESTE uses this idea and creates an arborescence called a “hierarchical descending classification,” which is illustrated by a dendrogram (see Figure 1). The relevant number of classes (or themes) is defined by the software. The classes are clusters of co-occurrences of particular words. As previously mentioned, co-occurrence is studied in text segments or context units. In our methodology, the context units are the answers given by each interviewee (they are short and correspond to the repetition of the same question); we did not use the random segmentation and double analysis proposed by ALCESTE. The co-occurrence of lexemes is determined statistically. Simultaneously occurring (co-occurring) words are given χ² association coefficients. The higher the χ², the less the allocation to a class can be due to chance. A class (or theme) is defined by a list of words that are strongly linked to this class. This allocation makes it possible to determine the answers with the strongest statistical link to a class (i.e., the most typical answers for a given theme). ALCESTE not only defines classes with which particular co-occurrences of words are associated, but it is also possible to associate indicators with them. For instance, by defining a rule that the context unit is the answer given by one interviewee to one of the three questions in the questionnaire (i.e., the elementary context unit [ECU]), a group of answers called the initial context units (ICU) can be determined, which shows whether the themes are associated with the proposed indicators. The lists of words must be placed in their respective contexts (the ECUs), but the meaning of the co-occurrence of words must also be found, as in component analysis. The designer of the ALCESTE program stresses that an analysis of this type can only provide working hypotheses: χ² is only an index and cannot be considered a probability. Spring 2010 87 Answers were analyzed in two steps, applying the method used by Defromont [14, 15]: • A primary analysis of the answers to the three questions provided information on the relevance of the three archetypes used. • A secondary analysis examined the composition of the three representations in more detail. Downloaded by [Laurentian University] at 19:45 18 March 2016 Results ALCESTE analysis of the text containing the 2,695 answers from the 904 interview­ ees yielded a four-branch dendrogram (see Figure 1) corresponding to four main classes. One of the classes corresponded to answers concerning the insane person, one to answers concerning the mentally ill person, and two classes concerned the depressed person. Three classes were obtained in the secondary analysis involving the answers to the question, “In your opinion, what is a depressed person?” All the classes are illustrated by lists of words with their respective χ² association coefficients. The words are put into context using the ECUs related to each class. Instead of providing lists of meaningless words, we prefer to describe the themes revealed by interpretation of the data. For a more detailed description, see Goodfellow [16]. For details of the ALCESTE analysis of the answers to the three questions during the first phase of the MHGP survey (1998–2000), see Defromont [14, 17] and Defromont and Roelandt [15]. The surveys were conducted in the Indian Ocean, France, and French Guadeloupe. We used Defromont’s work for comparison and to shed light on the results of this study. Primary analysis of the representations of the insane person (Table 1) reveal two themes in Nouméa; one is related to deficiency, and the other, to behavior. • The first theme in the social representation of the insane person is related to deficiency. It can be found with slight variations at all the sites. In Madagascar, the insane person is considered to be lacking reason and self-control; in Grand Comore and Mauritius, to have altered human functions, including reason; and in France, to lack reason, self-control, and responsibility. In Nouméa, the insane person has lost the ability to reason, to perceive reality, and to be aware of his or her actions. • The second theme in the social representation of the insane person describes behavior. In Madagascar, the behavior of the insane person is noticeable because it transgresses moral standards. In Grand Comore and Mauritius, the person’s behavior (and speech) is absurd and violent. In France, it is meaningless and incomprehensible and is also directly related to danger. In Nouméa, what the insane person does plays a central role in the definition provided by the interviewee. The verb does is overrepresented. The person does meaningless, strange, and unpredictable things, which imply danger. 88 International Journal of Mental Health Table 1 Representation of the Insane Person at Different Survey Sites France Downloaded by [Laurentian University] at 19:45 18 March 2016 Madagascar Grand Comore Mauritius 1st representation 2d representation Nouméa • Amoral, inde- • Absurd • Altered • Word insane • Strange and cency, nudity, and violent relation to the criticized: incomprehenrelated to filth, behavior world and to negative word sible behavior; wanders society to replace danger • Failure, defiaround mentally ill cient intellec- • Deficiency • Deficiency • Deficiency tual (intellectual) • Deficiency (loss) of rea(unreason(incapacity) son, of his or functions. able, not • Various origins: • Health-care in her sense of thoughtful) reality, of conmental fatigue, a psychiatric scious behavior troubles, drugs environment The central themes revealed in Nouméa were sometimes found at other survey sites to be related to the social representations of depression and mental illness, implying that the concepts are not well differentiated. The social representation of the mentally ill person (Table 2) is strikingly similar in France and in Nouméa. The mentally ill person has a disease, in contrast with the insane person who simply is insane. The specialized vocabulary used implies that the mentally ill person is comprehensible in medical, psychological, and psychiatric terms. The mentally ill person has become more accessible than the insane person, at least to specialists, and needs treatment and medication. Heredity is an important aspect in France and in Nouméa: mental illness occurs at birth; it is genetic. On the other hand, in Nouméa, the descriptions of the mentally ill and the insane do not overlap as they do in France and in surveys in the Indian Ocean. This difference may be due to the time interval between the first phase surveys (1998–2000) and the survey in Nouméa (August 2006). Jorm, Christensen, and Griffiths [18, 19] reported a major change in the attitudes of Australians toward mental health issues over a period of eight years, which could be true for the Noumean representations of the mentally ill and the insane. The descriptions of the depressed person in Madagascar and Grand Comore resemble the descriptions of the insane person (Table 3). In Mauritius, however, the descriptions of the depressed person resemble those in France and Nouméa (Tables 3 and 4). Three subcategories can be identified in the representation of the depressed person in Nouméa (Table 4). The first is a phenomenological category very similar to the humoral theory proposed by Defromont [14, 15, 17] in France: It is a descriptive category describing a state of mind or a disposition. The second category is also quite close to the humoral theory, although more explicative than the former category. This state is a medicalized category and describes a frail and Spring 2010 89 Table 2 Representation of the Mentally Ill Person at Different Survey Sites France Downloaded by [Laurentian University] at 19:45 18 March 2016 Madagascar Grand Comore • Abnormal or strange behavior and way of speaking • Deficiency: lack of conscience and self control • Very similar to the insane person Mauritius 1st representation 2d representation Nouméa • Tired and • Disease, • Insane person • Disease, use affected specialized and mentally of medical, vocabulary ill person very psychological, • Role of stress similar: altered psychiatric and alcohol as • Heredity, psyrelation to real- terms external stress chic/physical ity and to the • Heredity, cerefactors ambiguity world bral impairment • Must undergo • Danger • Moments of treatment lucidity ailing constitution explained by the “lack of something.” The third category can be compared to the nervous theory described by Defromont [14, 15, 17] but with two peculiarities: explosiveness and alcohol/drugs. The answers given in Nouméa to the question, “In your opinion, what is a depressed person?” were analyzed according to the different ethnic communities. The results of the analysis are not given here as the sample was too small, but they suggest that (a) phenomenological traits were more readily attributed to the depressed person by Europeans born outside New Caledonia and by Europeans born in New Caledonia; (b) constitutional and medicalized traits were attributed by Europeans born outside New Caledonia (born in France for the most part); (c) Europeans born in New Caledonia tended to attribute sadness, weeping, and explosiveness to the depressed person; and (d) Kanaks tended to attribute addictive problems, loss of control, and danger to the depressed person. This description resembles that given for the insane person and suggests that the representations of the two characters are not clearly individualized in the Kanak community in our sample. Discussion Beyond the limits related to the representativeness of our sample with respect to the general population of Nouméa and Greater Nouméa, the aim of this survey is to reveal the social representations related to exclusionary attitudes toward people suffering from psychic disorders. Sperber [20] stressed the importance of using an epidemiological type of approach to cultural representations, which are a form of social representation. He argued that the epidemiological type of approach is a complement to the anthropological type of approach, as it can identify variants 90 International Journal of Mental Health Table 3 Representation of the Depressed Person at Different Survey Sites France Downloaded by [Laurentian University] at 19:45 18 March 2016 Madagascar Mauritius • Abnormal • Fatigue, behavior loss of • Deficiency: courage, sadness memory, con• Troubles, science, alcohol, knowledge, drugs reason, control • Harmful events, trouble 1st representation 2d representation • Humoral • Nervous theory theory • Symptoms: • Nervous dischanging ease, loss mood, of control of pessimism, nerves bleak • Accumulaoutlook, tion/catharlack of sis. motivation, • Origin: emosuicide tional shock • External origin (personal or professional shock) Nouméa (general analysis) 1st representation 2d representation • Phenom• Social/ envienological ronmental • Sadness, • Gets worked weeping, up, has bleak outexplosive look, suicide outbursts • Nervous. • Sensitive, • Vanquished, drinks alcohol. isolation • Victim of his or her environment, family, or professional problems Table 4 Representation of the Depressed Person in Nouméa (Second Analysis) 1st representation Phenomenological/ humoral • Weeps all the time, mood swings • Sadness, bleak outlook, lack of motivation, suicide 2d representation Medicalized • Psychological and nervous disease • Fragile constitution • Lacks something 3rd representation Aggressive/ explosive • Gets angry/ has outbursts • Alcohol, cannabis • Family problems, needs help, needs to talk and invariants in these representations and help clarify what makes them vary (or not vary). In our approach, the investigation of social representations is based on the repetition of answers given by an entire population. This repetition was used to reveal the main trends that structure social representations. In our methodology, the opinion of the interviewee was respected and the questionnaire was totally anonymous: No one can be traced. This anonymity was important in ensuring the freedom of speech of the person who accepted the interview. The questions were simplified as much as possible so that they could be clearly understood. Spring 2010 91 Downloaded by [Laurentian University] at 19:45 18 March 2016 The Depressed Person in Nouméa: The Variant As previously discussed, the aggressive/explosive and addictive type of depressed person specific to Nouméa and Greater Nouméa intermingles with other types of description. The depressed person appears to be the most heterogeneous of the three representations. To shed light on these particularities, we should turn to the works of Salomon, an anthropologist working in New Caledonia, and colleagues [21–23]. Bensa and Salomon [21], who studied field data and New Caledonian legal archives to investigate the relation between Kanaks and the legal system, reported a close relation between alcohol consumption and violence in the Kanak community. The authors stressed the particular role of alcohol in Kanak culture and that it did not exist before the arrival of the first Europeans. In spite of colonial measures aimed at limiting it, alcohol consumption has reached considerable proportions since then. Consumption of alcohol is said to have originally been a mainly male activity related to manly values. One important finding of these authors was that the traditional Kanak authorities tended to consider alcohol consumption as benign and very often, if not an excuse, at least an attenuating circumstance for violent behavior, especially violence against women. Alcohol use/misuse was reportedly a frequent source of conflict with the French legal system. In another survey of female members of the general population in New Caledonia, Hamelin and Salomon [22] analyzed the relation between kinship and violence against women and the ethnicity related to violence within the family. This survey was based on the observation that in New Caledonia, the family is perceived as a source of protection and solidarity in a society that fails to structure individuals, which may be due to the absence of alternative socializing institutions. However, the survey was also based on the observation that the family represents a space where violence, especially gender-based violence, is created and reproduced. The authors reported that economic and communitarian cleavages often overlap and that the colonial history is still a very sensitive issue. The colonial past has reportedly led to an unbalanced sex ratio with a higher proportion of males, which could also explain why the representations have been profoundly marked by violence against women. Accordingly, the survey results showed that 14 percent of women interviewed had been molested by a relative in the 12 months preceding the survey, most of them being young Kanak women with no income of their own. The results suggest that adopted women are more vulnerable and that gender-based violence often continues from childhood and adolescence (18 percent of the women were sexually molested before the age of 18 according to all authors) to adulthood. In another survey of the general population investigating the relation between alcohol and violence against women, Hamelin and Salomon [23] reported social and cultural differences in alcohol consumption and in violence within couples. Concerning gender-based violence, young Kanak women living in rural settings were found to be the primary victims of violence, especially women living in the Downloaded by [Laurentian University] at 19:45 18 March 2016 92 International Journal of Mental Health Loyalty Islands. These authors observed that excessive alcohol consumption by women (six or more glasses on a single occasion, six times or more per year) is widespread and is a recent phenomenon related to social and cultural changes in the past few decades in New Caledonia. However, the excessive alcohol consumption by women is far below that of their male partners, which has a major impact on levels of physical and sexual brutality. Excessive consumption of alcohol by women was found to be related to psychological distress. The results of the epidemiological axis of the MHGP survey in New Caledonia described in the companion part of this article [11] reveal that the prevalence of mental disorders is higher in Nouméa and the Greater Nouméa than in France but that the overall distribution of mental disorders is similar except for disorders related to alcohol use, drug use, and post-traumatic stress disorder, which are more common in New Caledonia than in France. The observations of the surveys previously mentioned could explain the overrepresentation of disorders related to alcohol consumption, drug use, and post-traumatic stress disorder. They could also explain the characteristics of the depressed person specific to Nouméa and Greater Nouméa, where the depressed person is described as being subject to explosive outbursts, anger and is thus likely to be violent, drink alcohol, and to have family problems that he or she does not talk about. The family aspect in the socio-environmental dimension of the depressed person described by the Kanaks interviewed in our survey suggests a link with Hamelin and Salomon’s [22] survey on violence and kinship. The depressed person could be considered to be a vector of representations of psychological suffering with which any individual can identify without fear of being considered insane. This vector should thus become familiar and inherit traits related to social and cultural facts in the population to which the individual belongs. In our case, the depressed person encountered in Nouméa, especially in the Kanak community, could indicate the existence of a relation between the representations of alcohol use and violence, on the one hand, and psychological suffering, on the other. Our epidemiological method enables us to illustrate quite obviously the results of the socio-anthropological surveys we have mentioned. The Insane Person: The Invariant As previously mentioned, the perception of the insane person is international; that is, an insane person is defined in the same way all over the world. This definition calls to mind the works of Star [2, 3]. In 1950, she conducted a pioneering survey on a representative sample of 3,500 individuals taken from the general population of the United States. The aim of the survey was to analyze the place of mental illness and psychiatry in popular thinking. The method was the same currently used in similar surveys throughout the world. The three main themes observed were the following: (a) deficiency of intellect or loss of reason, (b) loss of self control, and (c) lack of responsibility for their actions. The behavior of the mentally ill was Downloaded by [Laurentian University] at 19:45 18 March 2016 Spring 2010 93 also perceived as not being appropriate in the context. Normal people are supposed to make their behavior comprehensible. If they cannot, their mental health can be questioned. Star concluded that the mentally ill were considered to have lost human characteristics, were completely dehumanized, and because of the loss of reason, self-control, responsibility for their actions, and their incomprehensible behavior, they were perceived as dangerous. Indicators of exclusion of the mentally ill were overwhelmingly present (two-thirds of the population interviewed). The mentally ill frightened them. Star concluded that although psychiatric knowledge was widespread in the population, fear was still very present because of the stereotypes revealed in her survey. She optimistically referred to it as a transition period during which advances in science and understanding had not yet had time to change perceptions and stereotypes related to mental illness. Fifty-six years later, perceptions are the same according to our survey. The American definition of the mentally ill person corresponds to that of the insane person in the MHGP survey. These definitions basically have two dimensions: loss or deficiency, on the one hand, and behavior, on the other. The former relates to the loss of, or deficiency of, human characteristics (e.g., intellect, conscience, reason, and control). The insane are not human; they are altered. This trait is related to the behavior of the insane who cannot be understood; their behavior does not fit any norm and cannot be recognized (i.e., it is unpredictable). The behavior is thus dangerous and, therefore, makes insane people dangerous (not to mention their loss of behavior control). According to U.S. interviewees in the 1950s, the mentally ill are not responsible for their actions, as the insane are not in the MHGP survey. Loss or deficiency appears to characterize the insane” There is no explanation for it: This is simply how it is. The behavior can be compared to a stigma, making them stand out from others, visible from afar. There is no need to talk to them to see that they are insane. This behavior also makes them dangerous, because they are unpredictable and incomprehensible. There is little reflection involved. It is more like a chain of reflexes, more automatic than reasoned, as it is not understood. The dimension of loss simplifies comprehension. Star [2, 3] already showed that this representation is related to a strong level of exclusion. The MHGP survey results suggest that the mentally ill person is a medicalized insane person. Supposedly, then, the mentally ill person should be less subject to exclusion, but this outcome does not appear to be the case. The cause of mental illness becomes physical and is consequently medicalized, giving medicine all rights in treating and controlling the mentally ill person. This definition explains how the population can identify with a depressive person but not with an insane person and even less with a mentally ill person. Conclusion Our results suggest opposing distributions of social representations concerning the insane person and the depressed person. One appears to be homogeneous and Downloaded by [Laurentian University] at 19:45 18 March 2016 94 International Journal of Mental Health possibly universal (i.e., insane person), and the other appears to be heterogeneous (i.e., depressed person). Social representations concerning the depressed person are matched the peculiarities of each cultural group, as if each group develops its own image of the depressed person using familiar and acceptable features that characterize the group when something goes wrong with one of its members. On the other hand, representations concerning the insane person depend much less on the cultural context. The designation of the person’s behavior appears to be a stigma. Intermediate elements can be observed between these two images and distributions. In our survey results, these elements were based on medicalized vocabulary. The description of the mentally ill in the MHGP survey is one illustration of this trend, even if it tends to be closer to a medicalized insane person than a depressed person. However, different transitional elements can also be found in the description of the depressed person. Using the image of the depressed person may prove to be valuable in prevention programs and in fighting the stigma attached to psychiatry and its users. Our results suggest that it is an acceptable character in the social representations of the populations we interviewed. As previously mentioned, the “depressed person” is familiar, human, a comprehensible character with whom we can identify without being afraid. This image opens the way for psychiatry and its users to become more popular and more acceptable in society, and possibly even a way to counter the exclusionary attitudes the users may suffer from. These images were used during the “Defeat Depression” campaign in Great Britain [24, 25] and the “Beyondblue” campaign in Australia [18]. Fighting stigma and exclusion is a concern for many other fields in our society, and other professionals and the users of psychiatry themselves have much to offer. If fear of the incomprehensible is international (or even universal) and fundamental in human disposition, then the fight against stigma and exclusion may never end. The represented object to which fear is attached may change at each step of the fight. The goal may, in fact, not be to eliminate inevitable fears but rather to redirect them. We have seen that the images of the insane person and the mentally ill person are too deeply anchored to allow modification. Identification is thus impossible, and trying to change these images would be useless. It is preferable to use the image of the depressed person, with which people can identify. For instance, when the interviewees of the MHGP survey who had a disorder detected by the MINI were asked if they had been cured for insanity, mental illness, or depression, the majority answered depression. This redirection does not involve stigmatizing new populations but rather fighting policies that tend to do so and questioning current stigmas. The point is to encourage flexibility in human thinking by questioning the tendency to remain stubbornly attached to particular attitudes. Note 1. See Star [2, 3], Link Phelan, Bresnahan, Stueve, and Pescosolido [26] and Masuda et al. [27] in the United States; Angermeyer and colleagues [28–32] in Germany; Jorm and Spring 2010 95 colleagues [18, 19, 33–36] in Australia; Paykel and colleagues [24, 25] during the “Defeat Depression Campaign”; Crisp, Gelder, Rix, Meltzer, and Rowlands [37] during the “Changing Minds” campaign, to which the Lancet dedicated a special issue [38–43], and Haghighat [44, 45] in England; Pénochet and Guimelli [4], Livet [46], and Gaussot [47] in France; and other authors worldwide [48–54]. Authors such as Elks [55], Stark Paterson, and Devlin [56], and Farrow and O’Brien [57] analyzed the way the media deal with mental health issues. Downloaded by [Laurentian University] at 19:45 18 March 2016 References 1. Roelandt, J.-L.; Caria, A.; & Mondière, G. (2000) La santé mentale en population générale: Images et réalités, présentation de l’enquête [Mental health in the general population: Images and realities, presentation of the survey]. L’information psychiatrique, 76(3), 279–292. 2. Star, S.A. (1955) The public’s ideas about mental illness. Chicago: National Opinion Research Center, University of Chicago. 3. Star, S.A. (1957) The place of psychiatry in popular thinking. Chicago: National Opinion Research Center, University of Chicago. 4. Pénochet, J.C., & Guimelli, C. (1996/1997) Représentations sociales, image du psychiatre et de la psychiatrie [Social representations, images of the psychiatrist and of psychiatry]. Nervure, 9(9), 28–37. 5. Jodelet, D. (1989) Représentations sociales: Un domaine en expansion [Social representations: An expanding field]. In D. Jodelet (Ed.), Les représentations sociales [Social representations] (pp. 47–78). Paris: PUF. 6. Jodelet, D. (1989) Les représentations sociales [Social representations]. Paris: PUF. 7. Moscovici, S. (1989) Des représentations collectives aux représentations sociales: Eléments pour une histoire [From collective representations to social representations: Elements for a history]. In D. Jodelet (Ed.), Les représentations sociales [Social representations] (pp. 94–103). Paris: PUF. 8. Kirch, P.V. (2000). On the roads of the winds: An archaeological history of the Pacific Islands before European contact. Berkeley: University of California Press. 9. Angleviel, F. (2005) Histoire de la Nouvelle Calédonie [History of New Caledonia]. Paris: L’Harmattan. 10. Barbançon, L.-J. (2003) L’Archipel des forçats [The convicts’ archipelago]. Villeneuve d’Ascq: Presses Univertsitaires du Septentrion. 11. Goodfellow, B.; Calandreau, F.; & Roelandt, J.-L. (2009) Psychiatric epidemiology in New Caledonia. 12. Reinert, M. (2006) La méthode informatisée d’analyse de discours “ALCESTE.” Application aux Rêveries du promeneur solitaire. [The “ALCESTE” software method for discourse analysis. An example of analysis on the “Reveries du promeneur solitaire”]. L’Encyclopédie de l’Astrolabe. Available at www.uottawa.ca/academic/arts/astrolabe/ articles/art0049/Alceste.htm, accessed January 25, 2010. 13. Reinert, M. (2003) Le rôle de la répétition dans la représentation du sens et son approche statistique par la méthode ALCESTE [The role of repetition in the representation of sense and its statistical approach using the ALCESTE method]. Semiotica, 147(1–4), 389–420. 14. Defromont, L. (2004) Les mots pour dire la folie [Words for insanity]. Revue Santé Mentale, 93, 46–48. 15. Defromont, L., & Roelandt, J.-L. (2003) Les représentations sociales du “fou,” du “malade mental” et du “dépressif” [Social representations of the “insane person,” the “mentally ill person,” and of the “depressed person”]. L’Information psychiatrique, 79(10), 887–894. Downloaded by [Laurentian University] at 19:45 18 March 2016 96 International Journal of Mental Health 16. Goodfellow, B. (2007) Le “fou,” le “malade mental” et le “dépressif” à Nouméa. A propos de l’enquête Santé mentale en population générale: images et réalités [The “insane,” the “mentally ill” and the “depressed” in Nouméa. Concerning the survey “Mental Health in the General Population: Images and Realities”]. Unpublished doctoral thesis, Université du Droit et de la Santé Lille II. 17. Defromont, L. (2000) Une approche des représentations sociales du fou, du malade mental et du dépréssif [An approach of social representations of the insane person, the mentally ill person, and the depressed person]. Ph.D. diss., Université du Droit et de la Santé Lille II. 18. Jorm, A.F.; Christensen, H.; & Griffiths, K.M. (2006) Changes in depression awareness and attitudes in Australia. The impact of Beyondblue: The national depression initiative. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 40(1), 42–46. 19. Jorm, A.F.; Christensen, H.; & Griffiths, K.M. (2006) The public’s ability to recognize mental disorders and their beliefs about treatment: Changes in Australia over 8 years Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 40(1), 36–41. 20. Sperber, D. (1989) L’étude anthropologique des représentations: Problèmes et perspectives [Anthropological study of representations: Problems and perspectives]. In D. Jodelet (Ed.), Les représentations sociales [Social representations] (pp. 115–130). Paris: PUF. 21. Bensa, A., & Salomon, C. (2003). Nouvelle-Calédonie. Les Kanaks face à l’appareil judiciaire. Un rapport de recherche [New Caledonia. The Kanaks faced with the judicial system. A research report]. Unité Mixte de Recherche no. 8128. Paris: Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique/L’École des Hautes Études en Sciences Sociales. 22. Hamelin, C., & Salomon, C. (2004) Parenté et violences faites aux femmes en Nouvelle-Calédonie [Family ties and violence to women in New Caledonia]. Espace, Populations, Sociétés, 2, 307–323. Available at www.eps.revues.org/pdf/195, accessed January 26, 2010. 23. Hamelin, C., & Salomon, C. (2005) Consommation d’alcool et violences à l’encontre des femmes en Nouvelle-Calédonie [Alcohol consumption and violence to women in New Caledonia]. Les cahiers de l’Ireb, 17, 177–186. Available at www.ireb.com/publications/cahiers/cahiers%20site.pdf, accessed January 26, 2010. 24. Paykel, E.S.; Hart, D.; & Priest, R.G. (1998) Changes in public attitudes to depression during the Defeat Depression Campaign. British Journal of Psychiatry, 173(6), 519–522. 25. Paykel, E.S.; Tylee, A.; Wright, A.; Priest, R.G.; Rix, S.; & Hart, D. (1997) The Defeat Depression campaign: Psychiatry in the public arena. American Journal of Psychiatry, 154(6 Suppl.), 59–65. 26. Link, B.G.; Phelan, J.C.; Bresnahan, M.; Stueve, A.; & Pescosolido, B.A. (1999) Public conceptions of mental illness: Labels, causes, dangerousness, and social distance. American Journal of Public Health, 89(9), 1328–1333. 27. Masuda, A.; Hayes, S.C.; Fletcher, L.B.; Seignourel, P.J.; Bunting, K.; Herbst, S.A.; et al. (2007) Impact of acceptance and commitment therapy versus education on stigma toward people with psychological disorders. Behavioural Research and Therapy, 45(11), 2764–2772. 28. Angermeyer, M.C.; Breier, P.; Dietrich, S.; Kenzine, D.; & Matschinger, H. (2005) Public attitudes toward psychiatric treatment. An international comparison. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatry Epidemiology, 40(11), 855–864. 29. Angermeyer, M.C., & Matschinger, H. (1999) Lay beliefs about mental disorders: A comparison between the western and the eastern parts of Germany. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatry Epidemiology, 34(5), 275–281. 30. Angermeyer, M.C.; Matschinger, H.; & Riedel-Heller, S.G. (1999) Whom to ask for Downloaded by [Laurentian University] at 19:45 18 March 2016 Spring 2010 97 help in case of a mental disorder? Preferences of the lay public. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatry Epidemiology, 34(4), 202–210. 31. Schomerus, G.; Matschinger, H.; & Angermeyer, M.C. (2006). Alcoholism: Illness beliefs and resource allocation preferences of the public. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 82(3), 204–210. 32. Schomerus, G.; Matschinger, H.; Kenzin, D.; Breier, P.; & Angermeyer, M.C. (2006) Public attitudes towards mental patients: A comparison between Novosibirsk, Bratislava and German cities. European Psychiatry, 21(7), 436–441. 33. Barney, L.J.; Griffiths, K.M.; Jorm, A.F.; & Christensen, H. (2006) Stigma about depression and its impact on help-seeking intentions. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 40(1), 51–54. 34. Cotton, S.M.; Wright, A.; Harris, M.G.; Jorm, A.F.; & McGorry, P.D. (2006) Influence of gender on mental health literacy in young Australians. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 40(9), 790–796. 35. Jorm, A.F. (2000) Mental health literacy. Public knowledge and beliefs about mental disorders. British Journal of Psychiatry, 177(5), 396–401. 36. Jorm, A.F.; Barney, L.J.; Christensen, H.; Highet, N. J.; Kelly, C.M.; & Kitchener, B.A. (2006) Research on mental health literacy: What we know and what we still need to know. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 40(1), 3–5. 37. Crisp, A.H.; Gelder, M.G.; Rix, S.; Meltzer, H.I.; & Rowlands, O.J. (2000) Stigmatisation of people with mental illnesses. British Journal of Psychiatry, 177(1), 4–7. 38. Heginbotham, C. (1998). UK mental health policy can alter the stigma of mental illness Lancet, 352(9133), 1052–1053. 39. James, A. (1998) Stigma of mental illness. Foreword. Lancet, 352(9133), 1048. 40. Jamison, K.R. (1998) Stigma of manic depression: A psychologist’s experience. Lancet, 352(9133), 1053. 41. Jones, A.H. (1998) Mental illness made public: Ending the stigma? Lancet, 352(9133), 1060. 42. Littlewood, R. (1998) Cultural variation in the stigmatisation of mental illnesss. Lancet, 352(9133), 1056–1057. 43. Porter, R. (1998) Can the stigma of mental illness be changed? Lancet, 352(9133), 1049–1050. 44. Haghighat, R. (2001). Towards a unitary theory of stigmatization. British Journal of Psychiatry, 178(4), 378–379. 45. Haghighat, R. (2001) A unitary theory of stigmatisation: pursuit of self-interest and routes to destigmatisation. British Journal of Psychiatry, 178(3), 207–215. 46. Livet, M. (1996/1997) La mauvaise réputation [The bad reputation]. Nervure, 9(9), 52–54. 47. Gaussot, L. (1998). Les représentations de l’alcoolisme et la construction sociale du “bien boire” [Representations of alcoholism and the social construction of “good drinking”]. Sciences Sociales et Santé, 16(1), 5–47. 48. Chung, K.F.; Chen, E.Y.; Lam, L.C.; Chen, R. Y.; & Chan, C.K. (1997) How are psychotic symptoms perceived? A comparison between patients, relatives and the general public. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 31(5), 756–761. 49. de Toledo Piza Peluso, E., & Blay, S.L. (2004) Community perception of mental disorders: A systematic review of Latin American and Caribbean studies. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatry Epidemiology, 39(12), 955–961. 50. Gureje, O.; Lasebikan, V.O.; Ephraim-Oluwanuga, O.; Olley, B.O.; & Kola, L. (2005) Community study of knowledge of and attitude to mental illness in Nigeria. British Journal of Psychiatry, 186(5), 436–441. 51. Hugo, C.J.; Boshoff, D.E.; Traut, A.; Zungu-Dirwayi, N.; & Stein, D.J. (2003) Com- Downloaded by [Laurentian University] at 19:45 18 March 2016 98 International Journal of Mental Health munity attitudes toward and knowledge of mental illness in South Africa. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatry Epidemiology,38(12), 715–719. 52. Ozmen, E.; Ogel, K.; Aker, T.; Sagduyu, A.; Tamar, D.; & Boratav, C. (2004) Public attitudes to depression in urban Turkey: The influence of perceptions and causal attributions on social distance towards individuals suffering from depression. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 39(12), 1010–1016. 53. van ‘t Veer, J.T.; Kraan, H.F.; Drosseart, S.H.; & Modde, J.M. (2006) Determinants that shape public attitudes towards the mentally ill: A Dutch public study. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 41(4), 310–317. 54. Wang, J.; Fick, G.; Adair, C.; & Lai, D. (2007) Gender specific correlates of stigma toward depression in a Canadian general population sample. Journal of Affective Disorders, 103(1–3), 91–97. 55. Elks, M.L. (1996) Popular press diagnoses and psychiatric diagnoses. Medical Hypotheses, 46(4), 331–336. 56. Stark, C.; Paterson, B.; & Devlin, B. (2004) Newspaper coverage of a violent assault by a mentally ill person. Journal of Psychiatry and Mental Health Nursing, 11(6), 635–643. 57. Farrow, T.L., & O’Brien, A.J. (2005) Discourse analysis of newspaper coverage of the 2001/2002 Canterbury, New Zealand mental health nurses’ strike. International Journal of Mental Health Nursing, 14(3), 187–195. To order reprints, call 1-800-352-2210; outside the United States, call 717-632-3535.